CHAPTER SIX (Part 2)
(Discussion: ... Continued)
Regardless of the modelling agent, it has been
suggested that the
exposure to violence in the family of origin teaches children
the
acceptability of violence (Kalmuss, 1984). It also
teaches them to
resolve conflicts through the use of violent tactics.
While
researchers agree on the strong modelling potential that
exposure to
violence in the family has on the perpetration of partner
abuse in
future relationships, the transfer of dysfunctional modes of
conflict
resolution to these relationships has not been formally
addressed.
These findings provide strong support for the
influence of
observing one's same sex parent hitting the other on the
perpetration
of current partner abuse. However, it should also be noted
that
exposure to violence in the family of origin was not reported
by
78.57 percent of males and 81.82 percent of females who
perpetrated
current partner abuse. The perpetration of partner
abuse by these
individuals can be accounted for in part by measurement error
as well
as by a number of other factors that may include the
modelling of
violence by peers and the media.
Past perpetrated partner
abuse.
Past perpetrated partner abuse
emerged as a significant main effect as well as an
interaction effect
with stress for predicting current perpetrated partner abuse
by
males. (A detailed discussion of this interaction
effect will follow
in the next section.) By comparison, the effect of this
variable was
not as salient in the prediction of current perpetrated
partner abuse
by females.
The above finding is supported by the results
of the correlations
conducted on past and current perpetrated partner abuse for
males and
females. The strength of the association between these
measures were
found to be greater for males (r=.46, p < .001) than they
were for
females (r=.29, p < .001). The magnitude of these
correlations are
consistent with other research that also surveyed respondents
over a
two to 2.5 year period. For example, O'Leary et al.'s
(1989) study
of stability in marital aggression within a community sample
of men
and women found that the correlations between past and
current
perpetrated partner abuse over a 30 month period were r=.31
for both
males and females. Marshall and Rose's (1990) study of
premarital
violence among college students provided similar results
(r=.33, p <
.001 for males and for females, r=.25, p < .001 for
females). These
correlations lend support to this study's finding that past
perpetrated partner abuse plays a role in predicting current
perpetrated partner abuse by both males and females.
The linkage between past and current
perpetrated partner abuse is
also supported by personality theorists who argue that
people's
behaviour is consistent across time and situations (Davison
& Neale,
1990). This line of reasoning aids in our understanding
of how
partner abuse is sustained and transferred from one
relationship to
another (Kalmuss & Seltzer, 1986). However, at the same time, the
relationship between these variables has been shown to be anything but perfect.
For many, current perpetrated partner abuse is not always predicted by past
perpetrated partner abuse and is likely to be influenced by a number of other
factors.
Stress
Although the relationship between stress and the perpetration
of
partner abuse has not previously been clearly delineated, the
results
of this research appear to shed some light on the how these
variables
may be linked. While this study's findings are
consistent with other
research demonstrating that batterers can be differentiated
by
measures of stress (Barnett et al., 1991; Neidig et al.,
1985;
Seltzer & Kalmuss, 1988), its results extend previous
findings by
examining the influence gender has on the relationship
between these
variables.
Analyses conducted on the individual stress
items revealed that
there were only two stress events common to males and females
that
significantly differentiated abusers from nonabusers (i.e.,
financial
problems and stopping school). While no other stress
events
differentiated female abusers from female nonabusers, male
abusers on
the other hand, were found to differ significantly from male
nonabusers on three other stress events (i.e., lost job,
changed job
and spouse started work). These findings suggest that
males'
experiences of a wider variety of stressors is more greatly
reflected
in the perpetration of current abuse than females'.
Analyses conducted on the full stress scale
provide further insight
into gender's influence on stress and the perpetration of
current
partner abuse. First, the significant interaction effects
between
gender and partner abuse based on both weighted and
unweighted scales
demonstrated that males who abused their partners during the
past
year had higher stress scores than females who did the
same. This
finding suggests that the influence of stress on the
perpetration of
partner abuse is dependent upon one's gender. Second,
the results of
Pearson correlations also lend support to a stronger linkage
between
stress and perpetrated partner abuse among males.
Results indicated
that while stress was found to be significantly related to
the
perpetration of current partner abuse by females, the
magnitude of
the correlations were half those found for males based on
both
versions of the stress scale.
The most definitive characterization of sex
differences with
respect to the relationship between stress and the current
perpetration of partner abuse emerged from the results of the
logistic regression analyses. Results clearly indicated
that while
stress is an important risk factor in current partner abuse
perpetrated by males, it is not one for females.
As a significant main effect, increments in
stress levels increased
the likelihood for current perpetrated partner abuse by males
by a
factor of 1.696. Analyses conducted on the male data also
revealed
two significant interaction effects (i.e., stress by past
perpetrated
partner abuse and stress by age). In each of these
interactions,
under high stress conditions, past perpetrated partner abuse,
and
being less than 50 years of age were associated with an
increased
likelihood of perpetrating current partner abuse.
Finally, a
comparison of the log likelihood ratio of the main effects
model, and
the stress and alcohol models showed that the stress
interaction
model provided the best explanation of current perpetrated
partner
abuse by males.
The influence of past perpetrated partner
abuse on partner abuse in
future relationships likens itself to that of exposure to
violence in
the family of origin, in that it is demonstrates a reliance
upon
pre-existing and well established patterns of conflict
resolution.
When accompanied by high levels of stress, a partner abuse
incident
represents a form of negative reinforcement (Bandura, 1973)
in which
the immediate source of stress is reduced following the
perpetration
of abuse.
If the pattern of current perpetrated partner
abuse is a reflection
of what has transpired in previous relationships, then it is
likely
that the contingencies described above may have been
operative in
those earlier relationships. When these contingencies
are repeated
over time, the result is a well established pattern of
behaviour that
is extremely difficult to extinguish. Albeit
dysfunctional, the
combined effects of high stress and past partner abuse
increases the
likelihood of current perpetrated partner abuse among males.
Results of an experiment examining changes in
the systolic blood
pressures of college students suggest that aggression could
be
adaptive (Hokanson, 1970). Findings indicated that when
subjects were
given an opportunity to counteragress following a planned
harassment-insult procedure, there was a dramatic decrease in
blood
pressure readings to prefrustrative levels compared to those
subjects
with no opportunity to aggress. Thus, perpetrating
partner abuse may
be for some males an adaptive response to frustration and
stress.
Once the partner abuse incident is ended, so is the stress
and
frustration that proceed it.
The interaction between stress and age
provides some interesting
insight into the dynamics underlying the perpetration of
current
partner abuse by males. An examination of this interaction
effect
indicates that for males reporting no stress, low rates of
current
perpetrated partner abuse can be found across all age groups.
However, for males reporting stress, those in the age group
35-49
followed by those in the age group 18-34 appear to be the
most
vulnerable to perpetrate current partner abuse.
Finally, males who
experienced stress and who were 50 years old or more,
reported the
lowest rate of current perpetrated partner abuse.
The above findings are better understood when
the items included in
the stress scale are considered. For the most part, the
stressors
included in this study reflect the concerns of younger people
(i.e.,
being fired, starting a new job, school, moving, having a
baby,
etc.). Post hoc analyses also indicated that males
between the ages
of 18 to 34 years who experience high levels of stress were
found to
have low scores on ego strength (r=-.28,p < .001).
In other words,
the stressors included in this study appear to be ego
threatening to
young males. It should therefore not be surprising that the
rates of
current perpetrated partner abuse are highest among those who
are
under 50 years of age and who report high levels of stress.
Alcohol Risk Factors
A comparison of Wave 1 and Wave 2 results demonstrate changes
in
the relationship between perpetrated partner abuse and
alcohol
consumption and dependence for males and females. In
Wave 1,
multiple regression analyses demonstrated that alcohol
dependence and
the interaction between alcohol consumption and the
neuroticism were
significant risk factors in male perpetrated partner
abuse. Analyses
conducted on Wave 2 male data found that these relationships
lost
their salience.
For females, the results of Wave 1 multiple
regression analyses
demonstrated that the perpetration of partner abuse was
predicted by
the interaction between alcohol consumption and EPQP
scores. The
findings of Wave 2 also indicated that alcohol's effect on
the
perpetration of current partner abuse by females was
dependent on the
influence of other variables. However, this time
current
perpetrated partner abuse by females was predicted by the
interaction
between alcohol consumption and observing one's mother
hitting one's
father, past perpetrated partner abuse and neuroticism.
Comparisons of Wave 1 and Wave 2 correlation
coefficients for the
perpetration of partner abuse and alcohol measures also
demonstrated
a diminishment in effect in Wave 2 for both males and
females.
Whereas alcohol consumption, MAST, SADD and the alcohol
dependence
index were all found to be significantly correlated with the
prevalence of perpetrated partner abuse in Wave 1 for males,
only
MAST and SADD attained levels of significance in Wave 2 for
current
perpetrated partner abuse. For females, only SADD
retained
statistical significance in Wave 2 whereas previously both
the MAST
and SADD were found to be significantly correlated with the
perpetration of partner abuse.
The diminished effect of the alcohol variables
on perpetrated
partner abuse can be explained in a number of ways.
First, alcohol's
diminished effect for males is better understood when the
effects of
attrition with respect to alcohol consumption are
considered. Given
that male dropouts consumed almost 30 percent more alcohol
than male
respondents who completed Wave 2, it is not surprising that
lower
levels of alcohol consumption are reflected in the
relationship
between alcohol and the perpetration of current partner
abuse. It
also reasonable to suggest that high alcohol consumers are
likely
among those who score high on alcohol dependence. If
so, the loss of
salience in the relationship between that alcohol dependence
and
partner abuse can be more easily discerned.
Although the instability of alcohol measures
is less pronounced for
females, it may also be affected somewhat by attrition.
For example,
Wave 2 female completers consumed more alcohol than female
dropouts
suggesting that low alcohol consuming dropouts could have
attenuated
the correlations.
Aside from the issue of attrition, the
relative instability of some
measures compared to others can be more easily explained for
both
males and females if one considers what it is that each
alcohol
instrument measures. For example, whereas the MAST
assesses the
social consequences associated with drinking (i.e., marital,
family
and work problems), the SADD and ALC3R assess its
physiological and
psychological effects (i.e., hangovers, blackouts and
inability to
control drinking). The lower MAST scores reported in
Wave 2 suggest
that male partner abusers in Wave 2 experienced fewer social
consequences associated with drinking compared to either
physiological and psychological effects.
Finally, the lack of association between Wave
2 EPQL scores and
alcohol measures for both males and females raises the
possibility of
yet another explanation for the diminished relationship
between the
perpetration of partner abuse and alcohol. The results
of the
correlations between Wave 2 EPQL scores and various alcohol
measures
suggests that alcohol's reduced effect on perpetrated partner
abuse
was not related to pressures associated with social
conformity.
Instead, differences in the relationship between perpetrated
partner
abuse and alcohol for both males and females may instead
reflect
actual changes in drinking behaviour and/or be reflective of
differences found in the reporting of partner abuse.
An examination of the interactions between
alcohol consumption and
observing mother hitting father, past perpetrated partner
abuse, and
neuroticism index scores reveals a number of complex sets of
associations. In all instances, females who did not
observe their
mothers hitting their fathers, did not perpetrate partner
abuse in
the past, and had low or moderate scores on the neuroticism
index
reported lower rates of current perpetrated partner abuse,
independent of the amount of alcohol consumed. On the
other hand,
the highest rates of current perpetrated partner abuse were
found
among females who scored high on neuroticism and who consumed
the
highest amounts of alcohol. This pattern however, was
not repeated
for females who either observed their mothers hitting their
fathers
or perpetrated partner abuse in the past.
In both these latter two instances, highest
rates of current
perpetrated partner abuse were reported by females who either
abstained from alcohol or consumed high amounts of
alcohol. The lack
of research on the relationship between alcohol and female
perpetrated partner abuse makes comparisons with other
studies
difficult. Nevertheless, the interpretations of these
results will
again borrow upon the explanations advanced by Frieze and
Schafer
(1984).
According to these authors, considerable
variability exists in the
effects of alcohol on women. Differences in women with
respect to
amounts of body fat, phase of the menstrual cycle, and use of
oral
contraceptives make it difficult to predict the effect that
drinking
may have on them. Because of this, the rate and level
of
intoxication may vary from one woman to another given the
same amount
of alcohol. Frieze and Schafer (1984) also suggested
that the
effects of alcohol in women may also depend on their
cognitive
interpretation of the physical sensations experienced when
drinking.
If the sensation of warmth associated with vasodilation is
interpreted as power (as is often the case for men), then
increased
aggression may result. On the other hand, if the
interpretation is
one of emotional warmth, the likelihood for violence is
reduced. The
unpredictability of alcohol's effect on women together with
the
cognitive interpretation of its effect help to explain why
the
influence of alcohol consumption on current perpetrated
partner abuse
by females with respect to past abuse and observing abuse by
mothers
is nonlinear.
It is also possible that for
high alcohol consuming females the
exposure to violence in the family of origin may also include
a
pattern of heavy drinking. Given individuals' genetic
predisposition
toward alcoholism (Brook & Brook, 1992), this hypothesis
seems to
have theoretical merit. While Brook and Brook (1992)
noted that
little is known about alcohol's genetic influences on family
interactions, post hoc analyses examining the relationship
between
"observing mother hitting father" and
"mother's MAST scores"
(assessed in Wave 1) revealed a correlation coefficient of
.18 (p <
.001). It appears that mothers who perpetrated partner
abuse
against their husbands also had drinking problems. This
finding
suggests that in addition to a possible genetic
predisposition toward
alcoholism, there may be the modelling of problem drinking as
well as
partner abuse by the daughter. It is possible that the
intergenerational transmission of partner abuse is also
somehow
linked to the intergenerational transmission of problematic
drinking.
Personality Risk Factors
The correlational and logistic regression analyses conducted
on the
personality measures with respect to the perpetration of
partner
abuse, have provided some of this study's most interesting
findings.
Logistic regression analyses conducted on the female data
revealed
that two of the personality measures found to be significant
predictors of partner abuse by females in Wave 1 (i.e., EPQP
and
Neuroticism Index) were also found to be significant
predictors of
current perpetrated partner abuse in Wave 2. This
pattern, was not
repeated for males as no single personality measure was found
to
predict current perpetrated partner abuse by them in Wave 2.
Measures assessing personality were found to
be more stable across
Wave 1 and Wave 2 for both males and females than was the
case for
the alcohol measures. The stability found among
personality measures
is consistent with the argument that personality is
biologically
determined, and therefore remains relatively fixed across
time (Buss
& Plomin, 1984; Eysenck, 1965). Given this premise,
the following
two questions are then raised: 1) Why did the relationship
between
the prevalence of perpetrated partner abuse and the EPQL (for
males),
ego-strength (for females), self-esteem (for females) and the
MacAndrew Scale (for males and females) diminish in Wave
2? 2) Why
did the Neuroticism Index (for males) and the MacAndrew Scale
(for
females) lose their salience as predictors of current
perpetrated
partner abuse in Wave 2?
With respect to the diminished effect of the
relationship between
perpetrated partner abuse and the EPQL, it is possible that
for males
who perpetrated partner abuse, their tendency to dissimulate
diminished in Wave 2 because they felt more at ease with the
interview process and questions being asked. The
lessening of the
effect for the ego-strength and self-esteem scales among
females may
have more to with changes in the reporting pattern of partner
abuse
in Wave 2, and therefore be reflected in a weaker
correlation.
Finally, the loss of salience in the relationship between the
prevalence of perpetrated partner abuse and MacAndrew Scale
scores
for both males and females may be associated with changes in
the
drinking patterns among males and females. Given that
the MacAndrew
Scale gauges an individual's predisposition toward alcohol
and drug
dependency (MacAndrew, 1965), the weakening of the alcohol
measures
reported earlier, may in part have influenced the strength of
the
association between the MacAndrew Scale and the perpetration
of
current partner abuse. Being that this is the first
study to examine
these relationships longitudinally, comparisons with other
research
cannot be made and as a result, limits one's ability to move
beyond
speculation.
The question regarding the loss of salience
for the Neuroticism
Index' for males and the MacAndrew Scale for females is a
more
difficult one to answer. As suggested previously, it is
possible
that changes in the response patterns for Wave 2 reports of
partner
abuse may have influenced the relationship between the above
described personality variables. Equally likely, is the
possibility
that changes made to the partner abuse models tested in Wave
2 have
also influenced the strength of these personality
measures. When
Wave 1 and Wave 2 partner abuse models are compared, the
following
important differences are noted: 1) the Wave 2 model was
based on
longitudinal data whereas the Wave 1 model was not, 2) the
Wave 2
model included all the variables tested in the Wave 1 model,
as well
as additional variables such as life stress events and
exposure to
violence in the family of origin, 3) the Wave 2 model
included
reported partner abuse from Wave 1 as one of its independent
variables, and 4) Wave 1 and Wave 2 employed different
statistical
approaches to test the partner abuse models. When all
these
differences are considered, changes in the relative
importance of
some variables from Wave 1 to Wave 2 are better understood.
Finally, the inclusion of past perpetrated
partner abuse in the
Wave 2 partner abuse model may have cancelled out the effects
of
individual differences previously reported for males in Wave
1. This
is supported by the significant correlation found between the
neuroticism index and current perpetrated partner abuse
(r=.16, p <
.01) derived from the Wave 2 data. Also recall that
Wave 1
regression analyses found a link between the prevalence of
perpetrated partner abuse and Neuroticism. It was also
this variable
(i.e., Wave 1 prevalence of partner abuse) that constituted
"past
perpetrated partner abuse" and was found to be a
significant
predictor of current perpetrated partner abuse by males in
the Wave 2
analyses.
In light of this linkage, one might expect
that the effects of
neuroticism on the perpetration of current partner abuse by
males may
have been suppressed and that the removal of past perpetrated
partner
abuse from the logistic regression model would improve the
explanatory power of personality. However, when this
was done, the
effects of personality still remained nonsignificant
suggesting that
its salience is likely to have been influenced by the factors
previously noted or others not yet considered (i.e., stress).
The relationship between personality and
current perpetrated
partner abuse in this study is more clearly delineated by
female
data. Results of the logistic regression analyses
indicate that high
scores on the EPQP and the Neuroticism Index were found to
predict
current perpetrated partner abuse. Furthermore, the
odds of
perpetrating current partner abuse increased by a factor of
1.333 for
those with high EPQP scores and by a factor of 1.355 for
those with
high Neuroticism scores. As was described for males
with high scores
along this dimension (Eysenck, 1965), females who perpetrate
current
partner abuse also have a tendency to over-react and
experience high
levels of anxiety. But in addition to these
characteristics, they
also possess the ability to be toughminded, uncaring and
antisocial
(Eysenck, 1965).
An interesting feature of the EPQP, is that
high scores along this
personality dimension are typically found among males
(Eysenck,
1965). Thus, the finding that high scores on the EPQP
predict female
perpetrated current partner abuse suggests that these women
are
characteristically more masculine. This premise is
supported by
Kalichman (1988) who reported that females found guilty of
domestic
murders scored low on the MMPI MF scale which indicates
higher
masculinity. In light of this study's findings of
equivalent rates
of current perpetrated partner abuse by males and females,
investigations into possible sex differences in partner abuse
might
prove to be more beneficial if they were directed toward
examining
differences in hormone levels (i.e., testosterone), rather
than
differences in gender.
The personality characteristics just described
are part of the
multidimensional profile of the partner abuser described in
family
violence research. Clinical research conducted by
Gondolf (1985)
found that male partner abusers experienced difficulties with
impulse
control, tolerance for stress and low self- esteem.
General
population (Bland and Orn, 1986) and clinically based
research
(Kalichman, 1988; Hale et al., 1988; Scheurger and Reigle,
1988)
found that male and female partner abusers exhibited
antisocial
behaviours similar to those characterized by the EPQP (i.e.,
social
nonconformity, chronic lying and trouble with the law).
The
challenge facing researchers is to determine the factors that
predispose individuals to different types of abuser profiles.
The association between personality and the
perpetration of current
partner abuse by females, and what is possibly an indirect
link to
current partner abuse perpetrated by males supports the
inclusion of
personality measures in general population based research on
partner
abuse. The diminished ability of personality measures
to directly
predict current perpetrated partner abuse among males also
suggests
that various characteristics may be subsumed within each
other. In
order to overcome this problem, developing strategies that
can
isolate the effects of each remains an ongoing test for
future
research.
Next: Chapter 6 Part 3
|